![]() |
If this is your first visit, be sure to check out the FAQ by clicking the link above. You may have to register before you can post: click the register link above to proceed. To start viewing messages, select the forum that you want to visit from the selection below. |
|
|
Thread Tools | Display Modes |
|
#1
|
|||
|
|||
![]()
Vultures are amazing *pilots*. The ones around the Magalies gliding club in
SA are known to range as far as the Kruger National Park, over 200 km away. I've flown with them at cloudbase as high as 14 000', had them formate on a wingtip and once over the shoulder of a Blanik, peering into the rear cockpit from less than a metre away. Flying in the Drakensburg one can often end up with 30 or more birds in a big thermal or fly in formation with a group of them along the cliff faces. Ian "Martin Gregorie" wrote in message ... On 04 Jul 2004 18:48:04 GMT, ospam (Frostowits) wrote: Pardon this input from an uninformed intruder to this subject, but why couldn't birds simply sense how strong the lift is by the amount of stress it puts on their "airframe". When I pump iron, I'm all too aware of the amount of effort required. Surely birds can do the same. Some seem to do just that. Kites in particular. I used to watch them a lot in India and discovered that you can tell how strong the lift is by looking at them - the stronger it is the more dihedral they use. If its really strong they just bomb round with a steep V-form and their tip feathers closed. Weaker lift gets more care and attention, less dihedral and more open tip feathers. When they're really scratching their wings are flat or even a little anhedralled and the tip feathers are fully spread and up to give tip dihedral. They initiate a turn with a big dab of negative in the inner tip and then control the turn on tail tilt - the outer tail tip is raised, so you can tell that they fly like we do with down force on the tail. Kites are easy to read because they often work low altitude lift where you can see exactly what they are doing and have big, long tails that are easy to observe. I wondered about how vultures fly but they were so seldom low enough to really watch that I couldn't work out very much. Also, with much shorter tails than kites its difficult to see whether they use tail tilt at all or which way its applied. The above is about all I know about soaring birds: I'm no ornithologist or naturalist. My background is chemistry, competitive free flight model flying and, latterly, soaring. I've heard a number of theories about how birds detect thermals including that they hear them. I'd well believe that, with a nerve on each feather, they must *really* feel the air and all its micro-turbulence. Maybe they can hear it too. However, that tells something about how they work 'normal' thermals but not a lot about how they can find and work the very weak, smooth lift you get early and late in the day. We know that migratory birds have a excellent directional sense so why shouldn't a soaring bird have a built-in vario too? I'd love to know how it works. If you haven't read it, find a copy of Philip Wills' "On Being A Bird". There's a chapter about flying with vultures in South Africa and how he worked just how vultures operate - altitudes, spacing, food finding strategy etc. The whole book is a good read too. -- martin@ : Martin Gregorie gregorie : Harlow, UK demon : co : Zappa fan & glider pilot uk : |
#2
|
|||
|
|||
![]() Martin Gregorie wrote: On 04 Jul 2004 18:48:04 GMT, ospam (Frostowits) wrote: Pardon this input from an uninformed intruder to this subject, but why couldn't birds simply sense how strong the lift is by the amount of stress it puts on their "airframe". When I pump iron, I'm all too aware of the amount of effort required. Surely birds can do the same. Well, the stress only occurs briefly on entry into the thermal, when you have upward acceleration. Once established in the climb, assuming it is more or less smooth, you no longer have acceleration nor any acceleration generated stress forces so you need some other means of knowing whether you are still climbing, such as rate of pressure change, which is what the vario measures. cheers CV |
#3
|
|||
|
|||
![]()
Martin Gregorie wrote:
On 04 Jul 2004 18:48:04 GMT, ospam (Frostowits) wrote: Pardon this input from an uninformed intruder to this subject, but why couldn't birds simply sense how strong the lift is by the amount of stress it puts on their "airframe". When I pump iron, I'm all too aware of the amount of effort required. Surely birds can do the same. Some seem to do just that. This doesn't work, as well as the earlier mentionnned feeling of vertical accelaration. This is because both only give just a "differential" information, i.e. not the rate of climb, but the change in the rate of climb. The real rate of climb can only been obtained from this information by a (mental) integration, but every such value in any domain obtained by integration from a raw differential input suffers a major deficiency: errors accumulate in time during this process so that if no other absolute information is available to determine the correct value at regular time intervals, the integrated value becomes meaningless. We all know that the feeling of accelaration is a good hint for a thermal, but we also check the vario to be sure it is not due to a high sink becoming very quickly a lower sink not deserving any circling. And while circling we check with the vario that the lift is not very slowly vanishing, at a rate at which no acceleration perception would warn us, and leave the thermal if this happens. ... They initiate a turn with a big dab of negative in the inner tip and then control the turn on tail tilt - the outer tail tip is raised, so you can tell that they fly like we do with down force on the tail. ... A raised tail (or elevator) doesn't mean there is a down force on it. On our gliders the opposite would rather be true : modern gliders are tuned so that that there is a zero force on the tail near the best glide angle of attack, in order to minimize the (induced ) drag due to this force, and as the airfoils used are unstable, at higher angles of attack we should have an upward force and a downward force at lower angles of attack, but the elevator is raised for higher angles of attack and lowered for lower ones. This is not a contradiction, it just means than without the move of the elevator, the upward or downward force on the tail plane would even be higher, so bringing back the angle of attack to its previous value, i.e. the tail plane fulfils its stabilizing duty. |
#4
|
|||
|
|||
![]()
Pardon this input from an uninformed intruder to this subject, but why
couldn't birds simply sense how strong the lift is by the amount of stress it puts on their "airframe". When I pump iron, I'm all too aware of the amount of effort required. Surely birds can do the same. Ted Frost Soaring Society of Boulder The stress would only change when acceleration was present (as in entering a thermal, we use our bums for for that - transitting from an area of intense sink to and area of lesser sink: e.g. no thermal). In a steady climb the stress wouldn't change and would be the same as in normal turning flight. I suspect that Martin's observation about the birds choosing a more extreme dihedral has more to do with lack of effort than sensing the lift intensity - a high dihedral result and a high value of lateral stability, but poor efficiency of the wing. Rgds, Derrick Steed |
#5
|
|||
|
|||
![]()
Can birds sense pressure changes? Maybe they have bio-varios.
I've noticed they seem to chirp, even on gloomy mornings, when I have noticed pressure rising on the altimeter or baro. It wouldn't surprise me if this was true, but I haven't heard of any studies... So I'm guessing maybe they know they are climbing from vario alone... In article , Derrick Steed wrote: Pardon this input from an uninformed intruder to this subject, but why couldn't birds simply sense how strong the lift is by the amount of stress it puts on their "airframe". When I pump iron, I'm all too aware of the amount of effort required. Surely birds can do the same. Ted Frost Soaring Society of Boulder The stress would only change when acceleration was present (as in entering a thermal, we use our bums for for that - transitting from an area of intense sink to and area of lesser sink: e.g. no thermal). In a steady climb the stress wouldn't change and would be the same as in normal turning flight. I suspect that Martin's observation about the birds choosing a more extreme dihedral has more to do with lack of effort than sensing the lift intensity - a high dihedral result and a high value of lateral stability, but poor efficiency of the wing. Rgds, Derrick Steed -- ------------+ Mark Boyd Avenal, California, USA |
#7
|
|||
|
|||
![]()
On 4 Jul 2004 20:39:04 GMT, Andy Blackburn
wrote: Any bird experts out there? While still in India I met a German lass who was an ornithologist and in India do a PhD on vultures. Naturally, I asked her about their flight performance. She just looked at me like I was a dinosaur: she knew nothing and cared less about their flight performance or operating methods. All she was interested in was stuff like population densities, diet and their behaviour when not flying. I found her attitude most odd. That was 25 years ago so things, hopefully, might have improved in ornithological circles. So, pick your bird expert carefully before asking about how birds fly. BTW, a good book about flight in general (literally from insects to 747s) is 'The Simple Science Of Flight' by Henk Tennekes. It won't tell you how raptors find lift but has a good analysis of how flying creatures size and weight affect their way of making a living and vice versa. Besides, any book on flight that can sensibly show everything from a Monarch Butterfly to a 747-400 on the same graph can't be all bad! -- martin@ : Martin Gregorie gregorie : Harlow, UK demon : co : Zappa fan & glider pilot uk : |
#8
|
|||
|
|||
![]()
I'm no bird expert, but did 20 years competing hang
gliding around the world, before really getting into gliding. Hang gliders mixed with (soaring) birds far more than sailplanes. Probably a performance thing. The birds seem to do different things depending upon the task at hand. IE: want to gain height quick - they'll be in the best core or thermal within the vicinity. Want to go somewhere- they'll be heading towards the next thermal that optimises their route. Want to 'hang out' they'll be in any old lift. I witnessed the whole USA hang gliding team joining a flock of circling Orubu in Brazil. They all landed next to the dead cow. Larger raptors can be more territorial and therefore don't have the same choice on area, thus might put up with inferior lift. Storks just seem to follow the leader. Swifts and Swallows are almost always in good cores, but I haven't a clue how they transit between thermals so quickly. The soaring birds not only find the best cores, but seem to very quickly know where the next thermal is. Many times I've watched their transiting direction - always dead straight - and altered my course to intercept or get ahead. It usually works. When we fly a lot, we pick up those extra sensory inputs - the micro turbulence, the twitch of the wing - it gives us huge amounts of info if we're open to it. By the end of the soaring season, I've sometimes just known where the core was by feel - which way to turn, etc. I don't know how - just intuitive. Birds fly all the time, so should be totally tuned in. Lastly, I'm sure they plot sink. We tend to focus on lift, whereas, of course, it's just a part of the equation. If you know where the sink is, then happy days, avoid it! Pete Harvey A falconer at the Parowan regionals last week told us that soaring birds have sensory organs that are able to measure the pressure differential between the outside air and inside their hollow bones. I have not been able to confirm this, but it would seem to make sense - think of the bones as capacity bottles. Any bird experts out there? |
#9
|
|||
|
|||
![]()
Take a look at Darryl Stinton's book "The design of the aeroplane" - there
is a captioned picture in the front of it showing a gull (an Albatross, I think) soaring a cliff in front of the camera. The caption points out aerodynamic function of various parts of the birds anatomy in a most enlightening manner! I don't have the book in my possession right now (I loaned it to the CFI of a local club, I should get it back I suppose) or I would post the picture and it's caption. Rgds, Derrick Steed On 4 Jul 2004 20:39:04 GMT, Andy Blackburn wrote: Any bird experts out there? While still in India I met a German lass who was an ornithologist and in India do a PhD on vultures. Naturally, I asked her about their flight performance. She just looked at me like I was a dinosaur: she knew nothing and cared less about their flight performance or operating methods. All she was interested in was stuff like population densities, diet and their behaviour when not flying. I found her attitude most odd. That was 25 years ago so things, hopefully, might have improved in ornithological circles. So, pick your bird expert carefully before asking about how birds fly. BTW, a good book about flight in general (literally from insects to 747s) is 'The Simple Science Of Flight' by Henk Tennekes. It won't tell you how raptors find lift but has a good analysis of how flying creatures size and weight affect their way of making a living and vice versa. Besides, any book on flight that can sensibly show everything from a Monarch Butterfly to a 747-400 on the same graph can't be all bad! |
#10
|
|||
|
|||
![]()
Our understanding of the reason/s for a bird’s excellent
soaring ability continues to be severely hampered by the lack of progress in establishing verbal communications. Perhaps a species that can fly through a thicket and sleep on a branch do not consider it worth their time. At 20:54 04 July 2004, Andy Blackburn wrote: Stress, or G-loading, is a measure of acceleration. As such it allows birds (or pilots!) to sense changes in the rate of climb, but not the climb rate itself (which we all know is a velocity, not an acceleration). I would imagine that birds can use these changes in acceleration to help find the center of a small thermal in some cases, but it might be less helpful in larger, more uniform bands of lift where the ability to integrate the cumulative acceleration effects over time is more difficult. A falconer at the Parowan regionals last week told us that soaring birds have sensory organs that are able to measure the pressure differential between the outside air and inside their hollow bones. I have not been able to confirm this, but it would seem to make sense - think of the bones as capacity bottles. Any bird experts out there? 9B At 19:48 04 July 2004, Martin Gregorie wrote: On 04 Jul 2004 18:48:04 GMT, ospam (Frostowits) wrote: Pardon this input from an uninformed intruder to this subject, but why couldn't birds simply sense how strong the lift is by the amount of stress it puts on their 'airframe'. When I pump iron, I'm all too aware of the amount of effort required. Surely birds can do the same. Some seem to do just that. Kites in particular. I used to watch them a lot in India and discovered that you can tell how strong the lift is by looking at them - the stronger it is the more dihedral they use. If its really strong they just bomb round with a steep V-form and their tip feathers closed. Weaker lift gets more care and attention, less dihedral and more open tip feathers. When they're really scratching their wings are flat or even a little anhedralled and the tip feathers are fully spread and up to give tip dihedral. They initiate a turn with a big dab of negative in the inner tip and then control the turn on tail tilt - the outer tail tip is raised, so you can tell that they fly like we do with down force on the tail. Kites are easy to read because they often work low altitude lift where you can see exactly what they are doing and have big, long tails that are easy to observe. I wondered about how vultures fly but they were so seldom low enough to really watch that I couldn't work out very much. Also, with much shorter tails than kites its difficult to see whether they use tail tilt at all or which way its applied. The above is about all I know about soaring birds: I'm no ornithologist or naturalist. My background is chemistry, competitive free flight model flying and, latterly, soaring. I've heard a number of theories about how birds detect thermals including that they hear them. I'd well believe that, with a nerve on each feather, they must *really* feel the air and all its micro-turbulence. Maybe they can hear it too. However, that tells something about how they work 'normal' thermals but not a lot about how they can find and work the very weak, smooth lift you get early and late in the day. We know that migratory birds have a excellent directional sense so why shouldn't a soaring bird have a built-in vario too? I'd love to know how it works. If you haven't read it, find a copy of Philip Wills' 'On Being A Bird'. There's a chapter about flying with vultures in South Africa and how he worked just how vultures operate - altitudes, spacing, food finding strategy etc. The whole book is a good read too. -- martin@ : Martin Gregorie gregorie : Harlow, UK demon : co : Zappa fan & glider pilot uk : |
Thread Tools | |
Display Modes | |
|
|
![]() |
||||
Thread | Thread Starter | Forum | Replies | Last Post |
Changes in Instrument Proficiency Check Requirements | Richard Kaplan | Instrument Flight Rules | 71 | June 10th 04 08:02 PM |
Kerry begins circling the drain: | Tarver Engineering | Military Aviation | 0 | February 12th 04 06:04 PM |
What determines LNAV "circling mode"? | Jack | Soaring | 11 | November 20th 03 05:15 AM |
Circling To Land On NDB-B and NDB-C | S. Ramirez | Instrument Flight Rules | 7 | July 13th 03 03:46 PM |